Many countries are enjoying a remarkable expansion in commercial air traffic as growing
populations, increasing wealth and the loosening of sanctions make air travel more attractive and
accessible. This growth is being fed by an ever increasing supply of the very
latest aircraft, principally from the two big manufacturers, Airbus and Boeing
– aircraft equipped with the most modern systems for navigation, communication
and engine management.
The manufacturers will argue that all this slick
technology will offer operators substantial savings with reduced fuel burn, better
aircraft utilisation and simpler, quicker flight crew transitions onto type.
They might also argue that some of the new systems make the operation safer by
adding layers of defence against mid-air collisions, flight into terrain or
unstable approaches. Both of these arguments are correct but ignore the
fact that in some cases new technology, or more accurately the way that we
humans interact with the technology, has been a factor in the evolution of
undesirable conditions and even accidents.
To illustrate this point it is worth going back a
few years to the previous generation of jet aircraft. If a system was not
performing as expected it was normal practice for the pilot (or more likely the
flight engineer) to switch it off and back on in an attempt to recover normal
service. Alternatively they might have pulled and reset the relevant circuit
breaker, of which there were multiple panels in the cockpit. However, that is
no longer necessarily the case. In a recent accident one pilot elected to
resolve an intermittent flight control system fault by resetting a circuit
breaker in flight, just as he had previously seen an engineer do on the ground.
Unfortunately, that particular system takes some time to recover after a reset
and in the meantime the flight characteristics of the aircraft changed to the
extent that the other pilot lost control and the flight ended in the sea.
In another case, one pilot made a simple numerical
error when commencing the take-off performance calculations and used a take-off
weight value precisely 100 tonnes less than the actual take-off weight. Partly
due to the process by which data was transcribed from the paper loadsheet into
a laptop electronic flight bag, and then back from the laptop to a paper flight
log and finally from there into the flight management system, this error was
never identified, even though there were four pilots in the cockpit at the
time. There were probably many other factors at work but the multiple
interactions with technology apparently ‘blinded’ the pilots to a substantial
and rather obvious discrepancy. The aircraft did eventually get airborne but
sustained major damage after scraping its tail along the runway and through the
overrun area before lifting off.
So why is it that technology designed to improve efficiency
and accuracy can sometimes have the opposite effect? Take-off performance
calculations are far more precise when performed by computer software as opposed
to the old fashioned tables and graphs. Electronic flight control systems
undoubtedly allow for smoother, simpler flying when they are working as
designed. So it is not the technology that is at fault, or indeed the concepts
that support it. Although in the first case described above there was a
technical fault with the flight control system, a cracked electrical solder in
fact, if the pilots had managed the deficiency in accordance with the abnormal
procedures they wouldn’t have fixed the fault but they probably would have
completed the flight.
Part of the problem is that we humans have evolved
to chase animals and pick berries and not to operate things with buttons and
levers – not yet anyway. Consequently our behaviours and thought processes are based
on conditions that are no longer very relevant to the environment in which we
work. Unlike just a few years ago, most of a pilot’s job today revolves around
monitoring and managing systems, the actual functionality of which he or she
would probably struggle to understand. It is sufficient in most cases to know
what the input options are and what the outputs can be expected to look like;
what goes on in between is of little operational significance. This is the
fundamental principle behind claims that pilot training has been simplified, (and
that can be interpreted as being quicker and therefore cheaper), by modern
aircraft systems.
That is true to a great extent but it brings with
it some unwanted baggage. Firstly this lack of understanding of what happens
between control selections and the eventual outcomes can become a liability
when systems do not function correctly or if conditions are encountered that
fall outside the design specification of the system. This was starkly
illustrated in a high altitude stall event a few years ago when the flight
dynamics were so far removed from what was deemed to be ‘normal’, that a
critical stall warning system intermittently suppressed itself, in accordance
with its design. The loss of the warning contributed to the confusion amongst
the pilots, who could not resolve a number of apparently conflicting pieces of
information. If they had fully understood the warning logic they may have had a
better chance. The investigation also determined that the pilots had not received
training in high altitude stalls, presumably on the basis that the systems
would protect them and they would never have to manage such a situation.
Secondly, these modern systems are so reliable
that deviations outside of ‘normal’ are incredibly rare. Now that should be
regarded as a good thing of course but as a result, and apart from the
controlled environment of a simulator, pilots might go many years without ever
seeing something significantly ‘abnormal’. This can lead to an over reliance on
the aircraft systems and a reduced ability to recognise any undesirable
deviations before it is too late to recover. Hour after hour of safe and
predictable flight may eventually be interrupted by a sudden and unexpected
flight condition and the industry is beginning to acknowledge the debilitating
influence of the ‘startle’ effect on pilot performance in an emergency.
This is not a Luddite’s case against modern
technology in aircraft; far from it. Terrain awareness systems alone have
dramatically reduced the number of inadvertent collisions with the ground and
bearing in mind the high fatality rates associated with those accidents, they
have undoubtedly saved hundreds of lives. No, the point is that as this new
technology sweeps through our commercial aircraft fleets, it radically changes
the way that pilots must interact with their aircraft systems, how they think
and how they behave. Manufacturers, regulators and operators must accept that
fact and ensure that the training offered to pilots – initial, recurrent and
upgrade – truly addresses the actual demands of the equipment they are to fly.
Jo Gillespie
Gates Aviation Limited
No comments:
Post a Comment